Recovered memory
Recovered memory is the description given to the apparent resurrection of the memory of events that had been forgotten or suppressed for a relatively long time. Retrograde amnesia secondary to physical or emotional trauma (i.e., traumatic amnesia), or the suppression of painful memories from any cause, is an accepted concept. But the mechanisms that lead to such reports are not well understood, and the authenticity of recovered memories has often been challenged; in some cases recovered memories are fictitious, although in other cases they may, apparently, be authentic.[1] [2] The term "recovered memory therapy" was coined by members of the 'False Memory Syndrome Foundation' to describe the process of recovering long-forgotten memories from people.[3]
The issues surrounding recovered, or false memories have sparked "one of the greatest controversies in the mental health profession in the 20th century".[4] Some therapists have proposed that memories of extreme trauma are buried in the subconscious by some special process, and are later reliably recovered. Others consider that genuinely traumatic events are seldom truly forgotten ("So far as the scientific evidence is concerned, traumatic amnesia appears to be a myth"). However, traumatized individuals exhibit a range of memory impairments, and research on children's and adults' encoding and memory of trauma stimuli has provided preliminary support for models of repression and traumatic amnesia.[5]
Authenticity
Those who doubt the existence of traumatic amnesia note that various manipulations can be used to implant false memories of traumatic events that can be quite compelling for those who develop them and can include details that make them seem credible to others.[6] Brown et al. state that “the hypothesis that false memories can easily be implanted in psychotherapy...seriously overstates the available data. Since no studies have been conducted on suggested effects in psychotherapy per se, the idea of iatrogenic suggestion of false memories is untested."[7]
One of the most famous experiments in memory research, conducted by Elizabeth Loftus, became widely known as "Lost in the Mall"; in this experiment, subjects were given a booklet containing three accounts of real childhood events written by family members and a fourth account of a wholly fictitious event of being lost in a shopping mall. A quarter of the subjects reported remembering the fictitious event, and elaborated on it with extensive circumstantial detail.[8] The study has been criticized as having "external misrepresentations" and "internal scientific methodological errors" as well as consultation and supervision issues.[9] It has also been critiqued as being misapplied to trauma memories and psychotherapeutic situations.[10]
Similar experiments were conducted by Porter et al., who found he could convince about half of his subjects that they had survived a vicious animal attack in childhood.[11] Some of this research of possibly creating false memories has also been critiqued in terms of applicability to therapeutic situations.[9] Another study has questioned the likelihood that a false memory of a traumatic memory can be created.[12]
The growth in the USA of recovered memory therapy for past sexual abuse has caused public and professional concern when memories are 'recovered' after long periods of amnesia, particularly when extraordinary means were used to secure the recovery of memory, there is a high probability that the memories are false, i.e. of incidents that had not occurred.[13] The hypothesis that false memories can be created in therapy has been questioned.[14] One study has shown that chronic childhood abuse is related to the development of amnesia for abuse memories; it suggests that psychotherapy is not usually connected to memory recovery and that the independent corroboration of these memories is often present. Cheit claims that the false memory movement has a "tendency to conceal or omit evidence of (the) corroboration" of recovered memories.[15]
Both true and false memories can be recovered using memory work techniques, but there is no evidence that reliable discriminations can be made between them.[16] Memories "recovered" under hypnotism are particularly likely to be false.[17]
Some studies have concluded that recovered memories can occur in victims of trauma and that memories of child sexual abuse and other traumas can be forgotten. There is evidence that traumatic memories can be recovered spontaneously, and in some cases recovered memories of traumatic childhood abuse are said to have been corroborated. [18] [19]
Sheflin and Brown state that a total of 25 studies on amnesia for child sexual abuse (CSA) exist and that they demonstrate amnesia in their study subpopulations.[20]
Betrayal Trauma Theory proposes that “that psychogenic amnesia is an adaptive response to childhood abuse” and that “victims may need to remain unaware of the trauma not to reduce suffering but rather to promote survival.”[21]
Medico-Legal issues
Serious issues arise when recovered but false memories result in public allegations; false complaints carry serious consequences for the accused. Many of those who make false claims sincerely believe the truth of what they report. A special type of false allegation, the false memory syndrome, arises typically within therapy, when people report the 'recovery' of childhood memories of previously unknown abuse. The influence of practitioners' beliefs and practices in the eliciting of false 'memories' and of false complaints has come under particular criticism.[22] Sometimes these memories are used as evidence in criminal prosecutions.
It is generally accepted that people sometimes are unable to recall traumatic experiences. The current version (DSM-IV) of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, published by the American Psychiatric Association, states in section 300.12: "Dissociative amnesia is characterized by an inability to recall important personal information, usually of a traumatic or stressful nature, that is too extensive to be explained by ordinary forgetfulness."[23] The term "recovered memory", however, is not listed in DSM-IV or used by any mainstream formal psychotherapy modality.[3]
Alan Scheflin, a law professor, explains that this satisfies courts that recovered memories are admissible into evidence in court. "Both those who argue that repressed memories are always false and those who argue that repressed memories are always true [...] appear to be mistaken. Although the science is limited on this issue, the only three relevant studies conclude that repressed memories are no more and no less accurate than continuous memories....”[24] A U.S. District Court accepted repressed memories as admissible evidence in a specific case.[25] The apparent willingness of courts to credit the recovered memories of complainants but not the absence of memories by defendents has been commented on "It seems apparent that the courts need better guidelines around the issue of dissociative amnesia in both populations."[26]
Neurological Basis of Memory
The neuroscientist Donald Hebb (1904 - 1985) was the first to distinguish between short-term memory and long-term memory. Things that we "notice" are stored in short-term memory for up to a few minutes. This memory is thought to depend on electrical activity in neuronal circuits, and is very easily destroyed by interruption or interference. Memories stored for longer than this are stored in long-term memory. Whether information is stored in long-term memory depends on its 'importance'; for any animal, memories of traumatic events are potentially important for the adaptive value that they have for future avoidance behaviour, and hormones that are released during stress are thought to have a role in determining what memories are preserved. In humans, traumatic stress is associated with acute secretion of epinephrine and norepinephrine (adrenaline and noradrenaline) from the adrenal medulla and cortisol from the adrenal cortex. Increases in these are thought to facilitate memory, but chronic stress associated with prolonged hypersecretion of cortisol may have the opposite effect. The limbic system, is critically involved in memory storage and retrieval as well as giving emotional significance to sensory inputs. Wihin the limbic system, the hippocampus is important for explicit memory, and for memory consolidation; it is also very sensitive to stress hormones, and has a role in recording the emotions of a stressful event. The amygdala is thought to assign emotional values to sensory inputs which are then elaborated upon by the neocortex and imbued with personal meaning.
Amnesia
Amnesia involves losses in explicit memory. It is shown by one’s inability to remember personal memories or discuss them verbally, or it may be shown by one’s inability to fully retain in conscious awareness temporarily retrieved memories. Amnesia is often considered to be a dissociative condition, such as dissociative amnesia. An individual may only remember parts of the event, or certain categories about the event (like feelings). Amnesia caused by deficits while encoding information may not be reversible, because the information was not encoded.
According to the defects in sequential processing theory, for a memory to become permanent, there needs to be a structural change in the brain. Amnesia is assumed to occur when the consolidation process is disrupted and either the memory trace doesn’t produce a structural change and gets lost or there is a structural change and access to the memory trace is lost. The multiple memory systems theories state that amnesia occurs due to a problem in one of the memory systems. These theories assume that the impairment of a psychological system may cause deficits in some kinds of memories but not others. However, none of these models can explain all aspects of amnesia.[27]
Effects of trauma on memory
Brain imaging studies suggest that trauma is associated with limbic system abnormalities. When stress interferes with memory, it is possible that some of the memory is kept by a system that records emotional experience, but there is no symbolic placement of it in time or space.[28] Traumatic memories are retrieved, at least at first, in the form of mental imprints that are dissociated. These imprints are of the affective and sensory elements of the traumatic experience. Clients have reported the slow emergence of a personal narrative that can be considered explicit (conscious) memory.
According to psychiatrist Bessel van der Kolk, memories of highly significant events are usually accurate and stable over time; aspects of traumatic experiences appear to get stuck in the mind, unaltered by time passing or experiences that may follow. The imprints of traumatic experiences appear to be different from those of nontraumatic events, perhaps because of alterations in attentional focusing or the fact that extreme emotional arousal interferes with the memory functions of the hippocampus.[29]
Mechanisms of interference
Traumas can interfere with several memory functions. van der Kolk [29]divided these disturbances into four sets
- traumatic amnesia; this involves the loss of memories of traumatic experiences. The younger the subject and the longer the traumatic event is, the greater the chance of significant amnesia.
- global memory impairment; this makes it difficult for subjects to construct an accurate account of their present and past history. In the sensorimotor organization of traumatic memories, sensations are fragmented into different sensory components.
- dissociative processes; this refers to memories being stored as fragments and not as unitary wholes.
- traumatic memories’ sensorimotor organization. Not being able to integrate traumatic memories seems to be the main element which leads to PTSD.
van der Kolk and Fisler’s hypothesis is that under extreme stress, the memory categorization system based in the hippocampus fails, allowing these memories to be kept as emotional and sensory states. Excessive arousal at the moment of trauma interferes with the clear memory processing of the event, leaving unaltered memory traces. When these traces are remembered and put into a personal narrative, they are subject to being condensed, contaminated and embellished upon. When traces are recalled, increased activity in the amygdala might cause the personal assignment of accuracy and individual significance.
van der Kolk states that trauma responses are bimodal. The response to trauma is hyperamnesia, over reaction to stimuli and reexperiencing the trauma, which exists with numbing, avoidance and amnesia. When compensating for chronic hyperarousal, subjects with PTSD may shut down and avoid stimuli similar to the trauma. PTSD subjects may go directly from stimuli to response without an adequate appraisal of the situation, due to their overgeneralization of incoming stimuli, creating flight or fight reactions.
Physiological manifestations of stress disorder
Abnormal physiological responses in PTSD have been shown in two ways. One is due to reminders of the trauma. The second is due to intense, neutral stimuli, such as loud sounds. Individuals with PTSD show several autonomic responses to these stimuli, like blood pressure, skin conductance and heart rate. These highly elevated responses show the timelessness and intensity of how traumatic memories may affect one’s present experience. Lang proposed that emotional memories are stored as associative networks, which are activated when a person gets confronted by a sufficient number of elements that make up these networks. Kolb proposed that excessive stimulation of the CNS during trauma could cause permanent neuronal changes, with a detrimental effect on stimulus discrimination, habituation and learning.[28]
Abnormal acoustic startle response (ASR) has been a main feature in trauma response for more than 50 years. Abnormalities in habituation are found in ASR with PTSD subjects. This failure of habituation for PTSD to loud sounds suggests problems with evaluating sensory input. The fact that PTSD subjects are unable to properly integrate trauma memories is shown physiologically by their misinterpretation of nonthreatening stimuli.[28]
PTSD develops following intense stressors. Intense stress causes the release of stress hormones, like cortisol, norepinephrine, epinephrine, etc. Constant exposure to stress changes an organism’s adaptiveness and how it deals with its daily environment. Studies have shown neuroendocrine abnormalities in PTSD subjects. These studies have shown chronically increased activity of the sympathetic nervous system activity in PTSD. Putnam’s work shows large neuroendocrine changes in sexually abused girls when they are compared to normals.[28]
Trauma victims do not respond to stress the way normals do. Pressure situations may cause a feeling of retraumatization. High states of arousal may promote the retrieval of trauma memories and associated phenomena such as sensory information or behaviors connected to prior trauma. Therefore, traumatic memories may be considered state dependent. Under stress, people secrete endogenous stress hormones that affect memory consolidation strength.
Memory processing in trauma
van der Kolk theorizes that there is a difference between traumatic and nontraumatic memory storage and retrieval. The body’s need to respond in danger situations can be strong. There is a tremendous physiological cost to this type of response, due to the depletion of hormones. When there is inadequate recovery time between stressful situations, alterations may occur to the stress-response system, some of which may be irreversible, and cause pathological responses, which may memory loss, learning deficits and other maladaptive symptoms. Children are very vulnerable to the negative impact of trauma, but if a trauma is strong enough, no person is immune to the possibility of developing PTSD. Animal studies show that learned helplessness can develop from repeated exposure to inescapable trauma. In humans, physical paralysis is sometimes connected to a traumatic event, with associated amnesia and dissociation. Traumatic events may be unavailable to recall or may be recalled only in pieces.[30]
Gaps in autobiographical memory are normal in PTSD, as are problems with short-term memory tasks. The successful coding of memories requires alert focused awareness when the input is presented. Memory consolidation is most successful when the experience can be elaborated on in conscious thought. A lack of conscious awareness may hurt these processes. Extreme, prolonged or repeated stress appear to interfere with hippocampal functioning. This interference may hurt cognitive assessment and the encoding of the input.[30]
Changes in hippocampal functioning during uncontrollable stress may limit the consolidation of the input into the explicit memory system. Some mental representations of the input may remain in cortical emotional memory, which may cause phobias and anxiety. This explains how trauma sufferers may have amnesia for specific events, but not the emotions connected to them. According to van der Kolk, in animal studies, memory is damaged when a situation can no longer be helped by the animal’s activity. Panic and freeze responses may be defenses to allow an organism to not consciously experience overwhelming stress or to not remember an occurrence of overwhelming stress. The second is by changing one’s interpretation of detachment. These events are characteristic of dissociative responses. These influences may cause memories unrelated to or dissociated from the normal methods of explicit memory retrieval.[30]
In animal studies, high levels of cortisol can cause hippocampal damage, which may cause short-term memory deficits; in humans, MRI studies have shown reduced hippocampal volumes in combat veterans with PTSD, adults with posttraumatic symptoms and survivors of repeated childhood sexual or physical abuse. Trauma may also interfere with implicit memory, where periods of avoidance may be interrupted by intrusive emotional occurrences with no story to guide them.[30] A difficult issue is whether those presumably abused accurately recall their experiences. [31]
Professional Organisations
The Working Group on Investigation of Memories of Child Abuse of the American Psychological Association presented findings mirroring those of the other professional organizations (see External Links subpage for references to various statements made independently by these organisations). The Working Group made five key conclusions:
(1) Controversies regarding adult recollections should not be allowed to obscure the fact that child sexual abuse is a complex and pervasive problem in America that has historically gone unacknowledged;
(2) Most people who were sexually abused as children remember all or part of what happened to them;
(3) It is possible for memories of abuse that have been forgotten for a long time to be remembered;
(4) It is also possible to construct convincing pseudo-memories for events that never occurred; and
(5) There are gaps in our knowledge about the processes that lead to accurate and inaccurate recollections of childhood abuse. [32]
References
- ↑ Geraerts E et al. (2009) Cognitive mechanisms underlying recovered-memory experiences of childhood sexual abuse. Psychol Sci 20:92-8. PMID 19037903
- ↑ Brewin CR, Andrews B (1998) Recovered memories of trauma: phenomenology and cognitive mechanisms. Clin Psychol Rev 18:949-70. PMID 9885769
- ↑ 3.0 3.1 Whitfield, Charles L.; Joyanna L. Silberg, Paul Jay Fink (2001). Misinformation Concerning Child Sexual Abuse and Adult Survivors. Haworth Press, 56. ISBN 0789019019.
- ↑ Loftus EF, Davis D (2006) Recovered memories. Annu Rev Clin Psychol 2:469-98. PMID 17716079
- ↑ See Policy Forum in Science (2005) and ensuing correspondence. Freyd JJ et al. (2005) Psychology. The science of child sexual abuse. Science (Policy Forum) 308:501. PMID 15845837 Quote from Kihlstrom JF et al. in response in Science 309:1182-5 PMID 16114120 with authors' reply
- ↑ Laney C, Loftus EF (2005) Traumatic memories are not necessarily accurate memories. Can J Psychiatry 50:823-8. PMID 16483115
- ↑ Brown, Scheflin and Hammond (1998). Memory, Trauma Treatment, And the Law. New York, NY: W. W. Norton. ISBN 0-393-70254-5.
- ↑ See Loftus E (1997) Creating false memories Scientific American 227 no 3 for a popular account
- ↑ 9.0 9.1 Crook, L (1999). "Lost in a shopping Mmall--A breach of professional ethics.". Ethics Behavior. 9: 39–50. DOI:10.1207/s15327019eb0901_3. Research Blogging.
- ↑ Pope, K. (1996). "Memory, abuse, and science: questioning claims about the false memory syndrome epidemic". Am Psychologist 51: 957. DOI:10.1037/0003-066X.51.9.957. Research Blogging.
- ↑ Porter S et al.(1999) The nature of real, implanted, and fabricated memories for emotional childhood events: implications for the recovered memory debate. Law Hum Behav 23:517-37 PMID 10487147
- ↑ Pezdek, K; Hodge D (1999), "Planting false childhood memories in children: the role of event plausibility", Child Development: 887–95
- ↑ Brandon S et al. (1998) Recovered memories of childhood sexual abuse. Implications for clinical practice. Br J Psychiatry 172:296-307. PMID 9722329
- ↑ Hammond, D. Corydon; Brown, Daniel P.; Scheflin, Alan W. (1998). Memory, trauma treatment, and the law. New York: W.W. Norton. ISBN 0-393-70254-5.
- ↑ An article by Cheit, RE (1998). "Consider this, skeptics of recovered memory". Ethics Behav 8: 141–60. DOI:10.1207/s15327019eb0802_4. Research Blogging.
- ↑ Stocks JT (1998) Recovered memory therapy: a dubious practice technique. Soc Work 43:423-36 PMID 9739631
- ↑ Kihlstrom JF (1997) Hypnosis, memory and amnesia. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 29:3521727-32. PMID 9415925 ("Hypnotized subjects respond to suggestions from the hypnotist for imaginative experiences involving alterations in perception and memory. ... Hypnotic hypermnesia refers to improved memory for past events. However, such improvements are illusory: hypermnesia suggestions increase false recollection, as well as subjects' confidence in both true and false memories. Hypnotic age regression can be subjectively compelling, but does not involve the ablation of adult memory, or the reinstatement of childlike modes of mental functioning, or the revivification of memory. The clinical and forensic use of hypermnesia and age regression to enhance memory in patients, victims and witnesses (e.g. recovered memory therapy for child sexual abuse (CSA)) should be discouraged.")
- ↑ Brewin CR, Andrews B (1998) Recovered memories of trauma: phenomenology and cognitive mechanisms. Clin Psychol Rev 18:949-70. PMID 9885769 (We outline four current explanations for the reported forgetting of traumatic events, namely repression, dissociation, ordinary forgetting, and false memory. We then review the clinical and survey evidence on recovered memories, and consider experimental evidence that a variety of inhibitory processes are involved in everyday cognitive activity including forgetting. The data currently available do not allow any of the four explanations to be rejected, and strongly support the likelihood that some recovered memories correspond to actual experiences. )
- ↑ Dalenberg C (2006) Recovered memory and the Daubert criteria: recovered memory as professionally tested, peer reviewed, and accepted in the relevant scientific community. Trauma Violence Abuse 7:274-310. PMID 17065548 ("Research during the past two decades has firmly established the reliability of the phenomenon of recovered memory. This review first highlights the strongest evidence for the phenomenon itself and discusses the survey, experimental, and biological evidence for the varying mechanisms that may underlie the phenomenon. ...an argument is made that false memory is a largely orthogonal concept to recovered memory; the possibility of one phenomena is largely irrelevant to the potential for the other... It is concluded that the weight of the evidence should allow the recovered memory victim to come before the court.")
- ↑ Sheflin, AW; Brown D (1996). "Repressed memory or dissociative amnesia: what the science says". J Psychiat Law 24: 143–88. ISSN 0093-1853.
- ↑ Freyd, J (1994). "Betrayal trauma: Traumatic amnesia as an adaptive response to childhood abuse". Ethics Behav 4: 307–29.
- ↑ Boakes J (1999) False complaints of sexual assault: recovered memories of childhood sexual abuse. Med Sci Law 39:112-20. PMID 10332158 '
- ↑ DSM-IV Diagnostic and Statistical Manual, American Psychiatric Association
- ↑ Scheflin, A (1999), "Ground Lost: the False Memory/Recovered Memory Therapy Debate", Psychiatric Times 16
- ↑ “The Validity of Recovered Memory: Decision of a US District Court” Judge Edward F. Harrington, Presentation by Jim Hopper, Ph.D. The legal documentation citation is: 923 Federal Supplement 286 (D. Mass. 1996), United States District Court - District of Massachusetts Ann Shahzade, plaintiff Civil Action No.: V. 92-12139-EFH George Gregory, Defendant. [1]
- ↑ Porter S et al. (2001), "Memory for murder. A psychological perspective on dissociative amnesia in legal contexts", Int J Law Psychiatry 24: 23-42
- ↑ Kolb, B; Whishaw I (1995). Fundamentals of human neuropsychology (4th ed.).. New York: W.H. Freeman.
- ↑ 28.0 28.1 28.2 28.3 van der Kolk, Bessel (1994), "The body keeps the score: memory and the evolving psychobiology of posttraumatic stress", Harvard Rev Psychiat 1: 253–65, DOI:10.3109/10673229409017088
- ↑ 29.0 29.1 Cite error: Invalid
<ref>
tag; no text was provided for refs namedVan1995
- ↑ 30.0 30.1 30.2 30.3 Knopp, Fay Honey (1996). A Primer on the Complexities of Traumatic Memory of Childhood Sexual Abuse - A Psychobiological Approach. Brandon, VT: Safer Society Press. ISBN 1-884444-20-2.
- ↑ Bremner, JD (2002). Does Stress Damage the Brain? Understanding Trauma-Related Disorders from a Neurological Perspective. New York: W.W. Norton and Company.
- ↑ Colangelo JJ (2007) Recovered memory debate revisited: practice implications for mental health counselors.(PRACTICE)Journal of Mental Health Counseling